BACKGROUND

Due to scientific discoveries and the rapid development of technologies, the borders between science fiction and reality are currently blurring (Harari, 2015). This phenomenon manifests itself in the possibility of technological enhancements of human beings, for example, in their cognitive skills, physical fitness, and endurance (Whitman et al., 2018). Human enhancement may be defined as a modification aimed at improving individual human performance and brought about by science-based or technology-based interventions in the human body (Coenen et al., 2008). Enhancing human beings has a long history, and its traditional forms are education (e.g., mnemotechnics), training (e.g., martial arts), and the use of natural substances such as caffeine (Bostrom & Sandberg, 2009). Examples of the therapeutic use of technology are prostheses and devices such as glasses, hearing aids, and cochlear implants, designed to bring people’s functioning to a level which is characteristic for the majority of the population (Bates at al., 2020; Daniels, 2000).

However, the distinction between therapeutic use and enhancements may be difficult to define (Erler, 2017), and the same technologies can serve both reducing deficits and increasing a given feature above the level typical for the human population (e.g., nootropic drugs which may enhance cognitive skills; see: Chadwick, 2008; Fahn, 2020; Whetstine, 2015).

In the future, implementations of human enhancements are foreseen, using advanced cybernetic, genetic, engineering, and pharmacological technologies (Jotterand, 2010; Lilley, 2013; Whitman et al., 2018). In this context, Pirmagomedov and Koucheryavy (2019) distinguish between three types of augmentation: physical, sensory, and mental. Examples of physical enhancements are exoskeletons and artificial arms and legs (Pirmagomedov & Koucheryavy, 2019). Sensory enhancements, such as infrared vision or sensing an electromagnetic field, may allow people to perceive naturally unperceived stimuli (Barfield & Williams, 2017). Mental enhancements may act, for example, by stimulating or weakening brain regions related to memory, emotions, or decision making (Barfield & Williams, 2017; Pirmagomedov & Koucheryavy, 2019). Currently, the issue of technological enhancement is discussed, e.g., in the context of developing brain-machine interface technology. On the one hand, it is presented as a method of restoration of sensory and motor function and the treatment of neurological disorders (Musk & Neuralink, 2019), and on the other hand, as a technology that may be used for mind and behavior control and to change the cultural concepts of privacy, autonomy, and self (Gurtner, 2021; Nakar et al., 2015).

According to Harari (2015, 2017), the technological enhancement of human beings seems to be an inevitable direction of the development of humanity. However, such enhancements may be assessed differently from the perspective of different versions of cultural narratives about human nature, traits, and destiny. Harari (2017) points out that three main types of definitions of humanism have emerged in the history of humanity. Liberal humanism emphasizes the freedom and uniqueness of the individual. In this version of humanism, every person is valuable, and the world is better when the more freely individual people can fulfill themselves following their preferences. Social humanism emphasizes groups and collectives. According to this version of humanism, the perspective of individuals is less important, people should associate and act for the good of communities, and collective needs should be more important than personal needs. Evolutionary humanism posits objective differences between people and recognizes that conflict is the basis of development. This version of humanism is related to the belief that humanity should develop at the species level, leading through systematic improvement and overcoming difficulties to the emergence of the “superhuman” species.

Technological enhancements of human beings are endorsed from the transhumanist perspective, which postulates the crossing of human biological limitations thanks to advanced technologies (Klichowski, 2015; Lilley, 2013). Kurzweil (2006) believes that thanks to technological enhancements of human beings, people will finally have the opportunity to fulfill their eternal desires for an endless, disease-free, and suffering-free life in happiness. From a different perspective, technological enhancements also raise several concerns. Tegmark (2017) shows that the close connection of biological aspects with technological and cybernetic inventions can result in various scenarios for the future of humanity. Schelle et al. (2014) indicate that social concerns about cognitive enhancements using pharmacological technologies raise three main issues: medical safety, coercion, and fairness. The safety aspect relates to the difficulty of predicting the long-term consequences of enhancements and the balance between personal benefits and costs. Coercion refers to possible environmental pressures forcing people to use enhancements to increase their effectiveness or social utility. The fairness aspect relates to the possibility of establishing or strengthening social inequalities through differences in access to enhancing technologies.

AIMS OF THE STUDY

Regardless of the actual possibility of the widespread use of technological enhancements in the foreseeable future, from a psychological point of view, an interesting issue is the study of people’s attitudes towards such enhancements that may result in their social acceptance or rejection. The aim of this study was to prepare a questionnaire for measuring attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings.

Another goal was to conduct preliminary research on its validity. For this purpose, we measured the relationships between attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and issues related to such aspects as personal values (Cieciuch & Schwartz, 2018) and beliefs about the role of science in the contemporary world (Jach, 2019, 2021). Additionally, we measured the relationship between attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and the level of acceptance for various versions of humanism indicated by Harari (2017).

According to Schwartz (1992), values are trans-situational goals, varying in importance, that serve as guiding principles in the life of a person or a group. The latest conceptualization of Schwartz’s model includes 19 values organized in a circular structure in which the coherent values are next to each other, and the divergent values are placed opposite each other (Cieciuch & Schwartz, 2018). An analysis of the transhumanist postulates (Bostrom, 2005; Klichowski, 2015) leads to the conclusion that technological enhancements are consistent with self-enhancement values such as achievement and power over resources. From a different perspective, the concerns about fairness, coercion, and medical safety indicated by Schelle et al. (2014) lead to the hypothesis about negative relationships between attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and the values of safety and universalism. Radical changes introduced to human life due to technological enhancements may also be inconsistent with traditional values (Lilley, 2013).

According to Jach (2019, 2021), the contemporary development of science and technology has led to the dissemination of the scientistic worldview, which can be defined as a form of worldview characterized by the tendency to justify one’s beliefs and behavior with scientific findings and to function based on theorems formulated by scientists. The scientistic worldview components are: trust in the scientific method, recognizing scientists as the only valuable experts, hoping that science will completely overcome problems that arise in human life, and considering science the best tool for exerting a practical influence on reality. A high level of trust in science and technological solutions is a common element of the scientistic worldview and transhumanist postulates. For this reason, a hypothesis can be made about the positive relationships of the mentioned worldview and attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings.

Although all three visions of humanism mentioned by Harari (2017) are very different from each other, they still constitute the context of the life of contemporary people. While Harari describes the types of humanism as cultural perspectives, it seems reasonable to assume that these types of humanism will also be reflected in personal worldviews. From this perspective, it can be assumed that attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings will be positively related to evolutionary humanism (emphasizing the importance of development and improvement) and negatively related to liberal (emphasizing the importance of freedom from coercion) and social humanism (emphasizing the importance of fairness and equal opportunities for all people).

PARTICIPANTS AND PROCEDURE

PARTICIPANTS

Due to the very recent characteristic of the research topic and its close relation to developing the new technologies, the study was conducted among young adults. Questionnaires were distributed among the respondents using the snowball sampling method. Participants could complete questionnaires in “paper and pencil” form or as an online survey. Participation in the study was anonymous and voluntary.

One hundred and thirty-nine Polish respondents (83 female and 53 male) participated in the study. The age of the participants was between 19 and 40 (M = 23.97, SD = 4.36). Among the surveyed participants, 2 (1.44%) had vocational education, 98 (70.50%) had a secondary level of education, and 39 (28.06%) had a higher level of education. Male and female participants did not differ in terms of age (t(137) = .66, p = .512) or level of education (χ2(2) = 1.75, p = .418).

PROCEDURE

Participants were first introduced to a brief description of technological enhancements of human beings and then completed the provided set of questionnaires. The definition of technological enhancements of human beings was presented to participants twice; for the first time in the introductory part of the survey (“The study concerns the relationship of psychological aspects with an attitude towards technological enhancements of human beings, that is, all forms of technological influences on people in order to increase their physical or mental capabilities”) and for the second time in the instruction to the Technological Enhancements Questionnaire (“Technological enhancements of human beings are all technical measures by which their users’ physical and mental abilities can be increased”).

MEASURES

The Technological Enhancements Questionnaire (TEQ) was developed to measure attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings (see Appendix A). The tool consists of 14 statements, each with five possible answers (1 – definitely disagree, 2 – mostly disagree, 3 – partially agree and partially disagree, 4 – mostly agree, 5 – definitely agree). For some items, the scoring is reversed. The content of the statements relates to such issues as the acceptance of the development of the technological enhancements, the readiness to use the technological enhancements, and the hopes and concerns related to them. The set of items included in the TEQ questionnaire was selected from a more extensive set of statements according to the opinions given by a group of judges familiarized with the concept of technological enhancements of human beings.

The Versions of Humanism Questionnaire. To study attitudes towards different versions of humanism, the Versions of Humanism Questionnaire was developed (see Appendix B). The questionnaire was developed based on Harari’s (2017) concept, distinguishing three versions of humanism: liberal, social and evolutionary. The tool consists of 25 items related to modern people’s life, for example, education, social life, art, and the role of the state. For each of them, the respondents choose one answer that best suits their views. The results are the sums of responses related to the liberal, social, and evolutionary version of humanism. The higher the score related to a given version of humanism, the more this version of humanism is accepted by the respondent. The set of items included in the questionnaire refers to the essential spheres of human life indicated by a group of judges familiarized with the concept of the three versions of humanism proposed by Harari (2017). The possible answers have been designed to reflect the differences between these three versions of humanism as accurately as possible.

The Views of Science Questionnaire (VoSQ; Jach, 2019, 2021) was used to measure attitudes towards the meaning of the scientific context in the contemporary world. The tool consists of 16 statements, each with five possible answers. The sum of the points obtained in all items is a score referring to a scientistic worldview level. The items relate to such issues as trust in the scientific method, perceiving scientists as the only experts, hoping for science to radically improve the condition of humanity, and considering science as a tool of practical influence on the surrounding world. Studies conducted so far have shown that scientistic worldview correlates positively with attitudes towards vaccines (Jach, 2020), esthetic evaluation of graphics depicting objects of scientific research (e.g., galaxies, neurons, fractals; Jach, 2020), and the perceived level of credibility of advertisements containing scientific references (Jach & Chmiel, 2018). Moreover, a study conducted by Jach and Buczek (2021) showed that the higher the scientistic worldview is, the greater is the acceptance of violation of ethical norms due to scientific development. The reliability of the VoSQ questionnaire in the present study was satisfactory (all Cronbach’s α coefficients are included in Table 1).

Table 1

Descriptive statistics and reliability of variables measured

VariableMSDMinMaxSkewnessKurtosisShapiro-Wilk W test p-valueCronbach’s α coefficient
Attitudes towards49.7310.411970–0.29–0.36.108.92
technological
enhancements
of human beings
Scientistic worldview49.3412.172080–0.03–0.14.596.89
Achievement13.053.33318–0.580.00< .001.82
Hedonism14.212.43718–0.50–0.05< .001.69
Stimulation10.943.56418–0.14–0.82.006.76
Self-direction15.212.26618–0.830.98< .001.79
in action
Self-direction15.252.20918–0.680.02< .001.74
in thought
Universalism-14.472.59618–0.890.67< .001.73
tolerance
Universalism-nature13.593.18518–0.710.02< .001.88
Universalism-concern14.373.01518–0.850.27< .001.76
Benevolence-caring15.222.30718–0.960.83< .001.78
Benevolence-15.452.33518–1.071.96.003.76
dependability
Humility11.013.03318–0.32–0.22.039.51
Conformity-11.093.86318–0.05–0.96< .001.89
interpersonal
Conformity-rules11.223.66318–0.20–0.60.014.87
Tradition10.144.23318–0.02–1.10< .001.88
Security-societal14.353.20318–1.000.89< .001.88
Security-personal13.552.63618–0.32–0.46.002.66
Face13.222.99318–0.860.95< .001.75
Power-resources9.753.533180.14–0.52.045.82
Power-dominance7.273.193181.041.26< .001.84
Liberal humanism (103)12.433.23519–0.15–0.51.033not applicable
Social humanism (19)7.912.551150.130.11.010not applicable
Evolutionary humanism (8)4.663.060171.051.32< .001not applicable

[i] Note. In versions of humanism, it is indicated in brackets for how many respondents a given version of humanism was dominant.

The revised Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ-RR). Participants’ values were measured using the PVQ-RR questionnaire (Cieciuch & Schwartz, 2018). The questionnaire consists of 57 items, corresponding to 19 values: achievement, hedonism, stimulation, self-direction in action, self-direction in thought, universalism-tolerance, universalism-nature, universalism-concern, benevolence-caring, benevolence-dependability, humility, conformity-interpersonal, conformity-rules, tradition, security-societal, security-personal, face, power-resources, and power-dominance). The higher the score on each scale, the more appreciated the given value is. The values identified in the model used are characterized by a satisfactory level of intercultural universality (Cieciuch et al., 2014) and are positively related to behaviors relevant to their daily fulfillment (Schwartz et al., 2017).

All presented statistical analyses were performed using JASP 0.14.1.0 software or Statistica 13 software.

RESULTS

STRUCTURE AND RELIABILITY OF THE TECHNOLOGICAL ENHANCEMENTS QUESTIONNAIRE

The frequencies of responses to each item are shown in Table A in Appendix C. Bartlett’s test and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin test were carried out to check whether the data collected using the TEQ questionnaire are suitable to use factor analysis. The results of both tests indicated the validity of the factor analysis: Bartlett’s test χ2(98) = 1038.80, p < .001; KMO test value = .914. Confirmatory factor analysis using varimax rotation revealed one factor with an eigenvalue above 1 (eigenvalue = 6.40; 45.7% of variance explained). The factor loadings ranged between .460 and .855 (mean = .666).

Subsequently, the reliability of the identified scale was analyzed: the standardized Cronbach’s α coefficient was .92, the mean item-item correlation coefficient was .45, and the mean item-scale correlation coefficient was .64 (for more information on the item-scale correlation coefficients, see Table A in Appendix C). The obtained results allow the TEQ questionnaire to be considered as a reliable tool to measure attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings.

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF MEASURED VARIABLES

Table 1 presents data on the descriptive statistics of the studied variables, their reliability, and distribution. Since among all measured variables, only the results concerning attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and the scientistic worldview were characterized by a distribution close to normal, non-parametric tests were used in further analyses.

ATTITUDES TOWARDS TECHNOLOGICAL ENHANCEMENTS OF HUMAN BEINGS AND DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SAMPLE

The male participants (M = 52.11, SD = 10.03) had more positive attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings than the female participants (M = 48.13, SD = 10.41): t(137) = 2.24, p = .027, Cohen’s d = .39. Respondents’ attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings were not related to their age: r = –.12, p = .162. The level of education of the participants was not related to attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings: t(135) = .70, p = .486 (participants with vocational education were not included in this comparison due to the small number).

CORRELATES OF ATTITUDES TOWARDS TECHNOLOGICAL ENHANCEMENTS OF HUMAN BEINGS

The relationships between the attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and the scientistic worldview, values, and versions of humanism were measured using Spearman’s rho coefficients. The results of the analysis are shown in Table 2.

Table 2

Attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and other measured variables – Spearman’s rho correlation coefficient values

Significant correlationsAttitudes towards technological enhancements of human beingsNon-significant correlationsAttitudes towards technological enhancements of human beingsNon-significant correlationsAttitudes toward technological enhancements of human beings
Scientistic.51***Stimulation.06Conformity-.02
worldviewinterpersonal
Achievement.17*Self-direction.13Conformity-–.14
in actionrules
Self-direction.18*Universalism-–.07Security-.02
in thoughttolerancesocietal
Tradition–.31***Universalism-.01Security-–.10
naturepersonal
Power-.18*Universalism-–.14Face.15
resourcesconcern
Liberal–.28**Benevolence-–.02Power-.00
humanismcaringdominance
Evolutionary.42***Benevolence-.09Social–.15
humanismdependabilityhumanism
Humility–.11Hedonism.16

[i] Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings were positively associated with the scientistic worldview and the evolutionary version of humanism. Lower positive relationships were also noted concerning such values as achievement, self-direction in thought, and power over resources. Negative relationships linked attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings with the value of tradition and the liberal version of humanism.

DISCUSSION

The obtained results allow the TEQ to be considered as a reliable, one-dimensional tool for measuring attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings. However, the analysis of the frequency of responses to individual items of the TEQ questionnaire motivates searching for its better internal structure. For example, in items 3, 8, and 11, the respondents presented very little differentiation of responses (e.g., in item 11, only two people chose one of the possible negative responses). In future works, consideration may be given to shortening the TEC questionnaire by removing these relatively weakly differentiating items, especially since even in the current study, their removal did not reduce the reliability of the questionnaire (see Table A in Appendix C).

The relationships between the attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and the values indicated in Schwartz’s circular model of values were partially consistent with the hypotheses. Attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings were significantly positively related to the values of achievement, self-direction in thought, and power over resources. These results suggest that the transhumanist postulates of technological enhancements may be more favored by those focused on independence, personal success, and using all available opportunities.

Attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings were negatively related only to the value of tradition. This result can be explained by the anticipated radical changes that technological enhancements may bring in the socio-cultural sphere. People who respect tradition may find it difficult to accept the need to redefine many of the concepts that have so far been appreciated. The negative relationship between attitudes towards technological enhancements and tradition is also consistent with the circular model of value (Cieciuch & Schwartz, 2018; Schwartz et al., 2017), where the value of tradition stands in opposition to those values which in the current study correlated positively with attitudes towards technological enhancements: achievement, and self-direction in thought.

From a different perspective, most of the analyzed values did not correlate significantly with attitudes towards technological enhancements. These results may indicate a vague understanding of this issue and difficulties in visualizing its more general consequences of technological enhancement of human beings. However, relatively weak relationships of attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings with values may also indicate that the values do not constitute a crucial context for potential human responses to the manifestation of such enhancements. This finding is a source of motivation to look for other psychological variables that may be more important from the point of view of attitudes towards technological enhancements.

The obtained results on the relationship between attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and values can also be compared with the declarative transhumanist values. According to Bostrom (2005), the core transhumanist value is having the opportunity to explore possibilities given by technological development, and derived values include individual choice, improving understanding, getting smarter, pacifism, pragmatism, caring about wellbeing, possibility to “tamper with nature” and acceptance of any form of diversity. On the conceptual level, the mentioned values seem to be in accordance with such values of Schwartz as universalism, self-direction, stimulation, and achievement, and in opposition to such values as tradition, conformity, and power. Some results obtained in the current research support the idea that conceptual values of transhumanism correspond with personal values related to attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings. However, the current study did not reveal correlations of mentioned attitudes with universalism, stimulation, and conformity, suggesting that the psychological background of attitudes towards technological enhancements may be different from the perspective presented in transhumanist manifests.

The positive relationships of attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings with the scientistic worldview indicated in the hypothesis were significant and the strongest of all measured relationships. This result suggests that people who trust scientists and the scientific way of describing phenomena may be more favorable to technological enhancements of human beings. From a different perspective, the obtained result leads to the conclusion that the large-scale development of the scientistic worldview may constitute the basis for future acceptance for the implementation of technological enhancements in human beings. In this context, the scientistic worldview may underlie the support for the acceptance of technological enhancements.

In line with the hypothesis, attitudes towards technological enhancements were positively associated with the evolutionary version of humanism and negatively with the liberal version of humanism. The positive correlation with the evolutionary version of humanism suggests that more positive attitudes towards technological enhancements may be associated with perceiving society as composed of people with various predispositions that can be assessed as “better” and “worse”. On the other hand, a negative correlation with the liberal version of humanism indicates that less positive attitudes towards technological enhancements may accompany the belief that every human being should be able to make independent choices and the belief that it is difficult to make a detailed evaluation of specific properties (e.g., cognitive).

However, there were no significant relationships between attitudes towards technological enhancements and social humanism, although the direction of the relationship was consistent with the hypothesis. These results could be related to the relatively small sample size, although the obtained results can also be interpreted in terms of ambiguity of opportunities and threats related to collective fairness postulates. On the one hand, in a situation of unequal access to technological enhancements, they may perpetuate interpersonal divisions, but in a situation of broad and equal access, they may lead to greater compliance with the perspective of social humanism. Recent international studies on public views of human enhancement technologies (Prudhomme et al., 2020) have shown that the most common is an egalitarian approach to the availability of technological enhancements.

LIMITATIONS

The most important limitation of the current study is related to the research sample – relatively small and collected using the snowball sampling method. Although this preliminary study showed good reliability and a unidimensional structure of the TEQ questionnaire, the psychometric parameters of the mentioned questionnaire should be measured in more extensive and more representative samples of respondents. Another limitation related to the sample is that only young adults were included in the study. On the one hand, the sample structure made it possible to interpret the obtained results in isolation from developmental aspects; but on the other hand, this structure made a more general interpretation of the results impossible. Future research using TEQ should include more demographically diverse samples.

Another significant limitation of the present study is the inclusion of a relatively small number of variables that can be considered indicators of the validity of the TEQ questionnaire. Although positive correlations with the scientistic worldview and several relationships with values (predominantly negative relationships with tradition) suggest that the tool measures attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings, future research should include a more detailed study of the validity of the TEQ questionnaire.

FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS

As mentioned above, the primary direction of future research using the TEQ should include repeating the present study in a larger sample. Future research may also include more differentiated psychological variables, which may be related to attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings, especially due to the relatively weak relationships of such attitudes with values. In this context, it seems interesting to include research on psychological wellbeing (e.g., Karaś & Cieciuch, 2017; Ryff, 1989), people’s perception of the functions of health (Górnik-Durose et al., 2020), and the functions of their bodies (e.g., Barreto et al., 2011). It could also be interesting to measure the relationship between attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings and aspects related to moral dimensions (Haidt & Joseph, 2007). The issues presented in the article can also be significantly developed through a more detailed examination of concerns about technological enhancements related to security, coercion, and fairness (Schelle et al., 2014).

CONCLUSIONS

The results of contemporary scientific research and technological development may lead to technological enhancements of human beings becoming a common phenomenon. Psychological research related to this issue can provide knowledge about variables related to accepting or rejecting technological enhancements. Such knowledge may soon be essential due to the possible impact of technological enhancements of human beings, e.g., in the spheres of work (Whitman et al., 2018), health (Academy of Medical Sciences, 2012), warfare (Latheef & Henschke, 2020), and education (Tilson & Aldridge, 2018), as well as its possible political (Blotget-Ford, 2021) and social (Lilley, 2013) consequences.

According to Jensen et al. (2020), Polish research on aspects related to human enhancements is at the very initial stage. However, the report of Prudhomme et al. (2020) showed that the majority of Poles surveyed would support technology that would extend human life to 120 years, increase the level of human intelligence, increase the level of morality and provide control over emotions.

In the context of the results of the above reports, it seems that a psychological tool is needed to study attitudes towards technological enhancements of human beings. The TEQ questionnaire presented in the article can be used as a short, reliable tool for researching the issues mentioned above. While this preliminary study identified several relationships between attitudes towards technological enhancements and such psychological aspects as values, scientistic worldview, and versions of humanism, future work on validating the questionnaire is needed.